Monthly Archives: January 2016

The Invisible Hand

We live in a market economy. We are frequently exposed to reminders of money. Does living under capitalism change our behavior? In a classic paper, social psychologists Margaret Clark and Judson Mills distinguished between communal relationships such as those that exist between family members and friends, and exchange relationships such as those that occur in business. Different norms apply to these two types of relationships. For example, people in an exchange relationship keep track of each other’s inputs into a joint task, while people in a communal relationship keep track of each other’s needs.

Several studies suggest that leading participants to think about money changes their behavior in predictable ways. These studies use cognitive priming to create subtle reminders of money. For example, participants may be asked to unscramble words into meaningful sentences. In one condition, all the sentences just happen to be about money, while in another condition they are about something else. In general, thinking about money increases achievement on difficult tasks, but decreases altruism or helping behavior.

In the latest contribution to this research, Agata Gaslorowska and her colleagues report four experiments done with Polish children aged 3 to 6. The priming manipulation was a sorting task. The children in the money condition were asked to sort 25 coins into three different denominations. Those in the control group sorted nonmonetary objects, such as buttons or hard candies.

Two of the experiments involved motivation and performance. In one of them, children who had handled money were more likely to complete a difficult labyrinth puzzle than those in the control group. In the second, those in the money condition spent a longer time working at what was essentially an insoluble task, a jigsaw puzzle intended for older children.

The other two studies involved willingness to help another child. In the third experiment, children were given an opportunity to help by bringing the child red crayons from across the room. Those who had sorted money brought fewer crayons than those in the control group. The final study measured self-interested behavior as well as altruism. As a reward for being in the study, the children were allowed to choose up to six stickers for themselves. Those who had handled money took more stickers. Then the children were asked if they would donate some of their stickers to another child who had not participated in the study. Those in the money condition donated fewer of their stickers. The results are shown below.

For each percentage of stickers donated, the graph shows the percentage of children in that condition who donated at least that percentage of their stickers. It should be noted that sorting candies put the children in a better mood than sorting buttons or coins, but mood was unrelated to helping in this experiment.

These experiments show that thinking about money affects the behavior of 3 to 6-year-old children in ways that are similar to its effects on adults. These kids had only a limited understanding of money. For example, they were unable to identify, at better than chance, which coin would buy the most candy. Nevertheless, they were aware enough of the function of money for it to change their behavior.

One of the authors of the study, Kathleen Vohs, proposes that the unifying thread in all these money studies is that thinking about money causes people to place a greater value on self-sufficiency. In another of her studies, adults primed with thoughts of money were more likely to choose to work alone rather than with another participant. If it’s good to be self-sufficient, this could explain why people in need are seen as less deserving of help.

Sociologist Robert Putnam, in his book Bowling Alone, presents data suggesting that over the last 50 years, Americans have engaged in fewer group and community activities and more solitary ones, with the result that we are less cooperative and trusting. Ironically, Putnam uses a market metaphor to summarize his theory. He says the disintegration of communal relationships reduces social capital, giving society fewer resources that can be used for the public good in times of need.

Michael Sandel, a political philosopher, argues that we have gone from having a market economy to being a market society. Public goods are increasingly privatized and virtually everything is for sale if the price is right. He summarizes his critique in this TED talk.

Since most of us have never lived under any other economic system, we are largely unaware of how capitalism affects our behavior. However, some of us spend more time handling and thinking about money than others. In one study, college students majoring in economics behaved less cooperatively in a bargaining game than students majoring in other fields. Studies consistently show that poor people are more generous and helpful than rich people.

These studies have something to appeal to people of all political persuasions. Conservatives will no doubt be pleased to learn that thinking about money encourages hard work and achievement. On the other hand, the finding that the market society replaces helpfulness with selfishness confirms an important part of the liberal critique of capitalism.

You may also be interested in reading:

More Bad News For Religion

On Obama’s Speech

Power and Corruption, Part 1

Trump’s Trump Card

Kenneth MacWilliams, a pollster and graduate student at the University of Massachusetts, reports that only two variables predict support for Donald Trump among Republican voters. Gender, age, income, education, religiosity and even ideology failed to predict Trump support. The two significant predictors were authoritarianism and fear of terrorism, and authoritarianism was “far more significant.”

MacWilliams’ article is light on details, but the poll was a national sample of 1800 registered voters conducted by UMass during the last five days of December.

What is authoritarianism? The theory of the authoritarian personality has its origin in the aftermath of World War II when social scientists were attempting to account for anti-Semitism in Europe. It was originally measured using the California F-Scale, in which “F” stands for fascism.

The most extensive research program on authoritarianism was conducted by Bob Altemeyer of the University of Manitoba in the 1980s and 1990s. He found that authoritarianism is best described by three attitudinal clusters:

  • Authoritarian submission refers to a high degree of obedience to authorities that are regarded as legitimate in the society in which you live.
  • Authoritarian aggression refers to hostile behavior directed at disliked outgroups, provided that such aggression is sanctioned by authorities.
  • Conventionalism refers to a high degree of conformity to behavioral norms endorsed by religious and political authorities.

Combining the first two clusters, authoritarians are said to have a bicyclist’s personality. They bow to those they perceive to be above them in the social structure, while kicking those they think are below them. Not surprisingly, people high in authoritarianism tend to be politically conservative, religious, and prejudiced against racial and ethnic minorities and homosexuals. They favor more punitive sentences for criminals and are more accepting of covert government surveillance such as illegal wiretaps. Their preferences for strong leaders and for the exclusion of outsiders are consistent with their support for Trump. MacWilliams found that high authoritarians were more likely to support deporting immigrants that are in the country illegally, prohibiting Muslims from entering the country, closing mosques, and establishing a national data base to track all Muslims.

MacWilliams measured authoritarianism with four questions about child rearing. Participants were asked whether it is more important for children to be respectful or independent, obedient or self-reliant, well-behaved or considerate, and well-mannered or curious. The first of each pair is the authoritarian option. While these questions may seem remote from politics, I see this as a strength of the current survey, since these items are largely independent of any campaign issues.

Political scientists Marc Hetherington and Jonathan Weiler found that, while at one time authoritarianism was unrelated to party affiliation, over the last several decades white authoritarians have gravitated to the Republican party while non-authoritarians have shifted into the Democratic party. This may be a result of the Democrats’ support for civil rights and Republicans’ “Southern strategy” of using coded racial messages to appeal to white Southern voters. In the current survey, 49% of Republican voters scored among the top quarter of authoritarians, over twice as many as the number of Democratics.

In 2008, authoritarianism predicted preference for Hillary Clinton over Barack Obama among Democratic voters. However, in the current survey, authoritarianism did not predict Democrats’ candidate preference. (Maybe not enough Democrats are aware that Bernie Sanders is Jewish yet.)

Hetherington and Suhay found that the threat of terrorism is associated with greater support for an aggressive foreign policy and the suspension of civil liberties among low authoritarians, but not among high authoritarians, since they prefer these policies regardless of the threat level. In other words, the threat of terrorism leads low authoritarians to act like high authoritarians. There is a very real danger that terrorist attacks in the U. S. and Europe could influence the 2016 presidential election.

Figure-3-Threat-Decreases-Effect-of-Authoritarianism-on-Preference-for-Military-Strength

Altemeyer reports a small study in which he had two groups of about 65 participants each—one consisting of high authoritarians and the other of low authoritarians—play the Global Change Game, a complex 3-hour simulation of the Earth’s future in which players represent different continents. In the low authoritarian simulation, no wars or threats of wars occurred and there was considerable international cooperation. However, the the high authoritarian game, countries responded to the same crises by increasing their arms and the session ended with a nuclear war in which the total population of the Earth was declared dead.

Of course, it was only a game.

You may also be interested in reading:

The World According to the Donald

A Darker Side of Politics

Old-Fashioned Racism